Tuesday, October 25, 2016

Statistical process control of color difference data, part 2

Last week, some stark raving mad heretic grabbed my blogging pen, spouting out some blasphemy about how the classical approach to process control is doomed to fail for color difference data. Asteroids laying waste to heavily populated areas, cats sleeping with dogs, my local Starbucks being out of chai... all that doomsday stuff.

Well, perhaps the guy who was using my blogging pen wasn't stark raving mad. Maybe he was just stark raving "mildly annoyed"? And maybe the heretic wasn't just some other guy? I don't want to point the finger, but it might have been me who wrote the blog post. So, perhaps I need to take his contentious assertion seriously?

Here are the sacrilegious assertions from last week's blog post:

Part 1 - Color difference data does not fit a Normal Distribution.
Part 2 - Classical SPC is largely based on the assumption of normality, so much of it does not work well for color difference data.

I submit the chart below as evidence for the first assertion.

This is not normal data!

I need to give some provenance for this data.

In 2006, the SNAP committee (Specifications for Newspaper Advertising Production) took on a large project to come to some consensus about what color you get when you mix specific quantities of CMYK ink on newsprint. A total of 102 newspapers printed a test form on its presses. The test form had 928 color patches. All of the test forms were measured by one very busy spectrophotometer. The data was averaged by patch type, and it became known as CGATS TR 002.

For this blog post, I had a close look at the original data. For each of the 928 patches and for each of the 102 printers, I compared the average L*a*b* value against the measured L*a*b* value. As a result, I had just short of 100K color difference values (in ΔE00).

Of the 94,656 color differences, there were 1,392 that were between 0.0 ΔE00 and 0.5 ΔE00. There were 7,095 between 0.5 ΔE00 and 1.0 ΔE00. And so on. The blue bars in the above chart are a histogram of this color difference data.

I computed the mean and standard deviation of the color difference data: 2.93, and 1.78, respectively. The orange line in the above chart is a normal distribution with those values. Now, we all like to think our data is normal. We all like to think that our data doesn't skew to the right or to the left. The bad news for this election season is that our color difference data is not normal. It is decidedly skewed to the left. (I provide no comment on whether other data in this election season is skewed either to the right or to the left.)

The coefficient of skewness of this distribution is about 1.0, which is about 125 times the skewness that one might expect from a normal distribution. "The data is skewed, Jim!"

The data is skewed, Jim!

Ok. So Bones tells us the data is skewed?  Someone may argue that I have committed the statistical equivalent of a venial sin. True. I combined apples and oranges. When I computed the color differences, I was comparing apples to apples, but then I piled all the apple differences and all the orange differences into one big pile. Is there some reason to put the variation of solid cyan patches in the same box as the variation of 50% magenta patches?

Just to check that, I pulled out the patches individually, and did the skewness test on each of the 928 sets of data. Sorry, nit pickers. Same results. "The data is still skewed, Jim!"

The data is still skewed, Jim!

Yeah, but who cares?  The whole classical process control thing will still work out, right? Well.... maybe. Kinda maybe. Or, kinda maybe probably not.

I looked once again at the data set. For each of the 928 patches, I computed the 3 sigma upper limit for color difference data. Then I counted outliers. Before I go on, I will come up with a prediction of how many outliers we expect to see.

One would think that the folks doing these 102 press runs were reasonably diligent in the operation of the press for these press runs. The companies all volunteered their time, press time, and materials to this endeavor, so presumably they cared about getting good results. I think it is reasonable to assume that on the whole, they upped their game, if only a little bit just to humor the boss.

Further, back in 2006, several people (myself included) blessed the data. No one could come up with any strong reason to remove any of the individual data points.

So, I am going to state that the variation in the data set should be almost entirely "common cause" variation. This is the inevitable variation that we will see out of any process. Now, let's review the blog post of an extremely gifted and bashful applied mathematician and color scientist. Last week, I wrote the following:

If the process produces normal data, and if nothing changes in our process, then 99.74% of the time, the part will be within those control limits. And once every 400 parts, we will find a part that is nothing more than an unavoidable statistical anomaly.

There were 94,656 data points, and we expect 0.26% outliers... that would put the expectation at about 249 outliers in the whole bunch. Drum roll, please... I found 938! For this data set, I found four times as many outliers as expected.

To put this in practical terms, if a plant were to have followed traditional statistical process control methods on this set of color difference data, they would be shutting down the presses to check it's operation four times as often as they really should. This is a waste of time and money, and as Deming would tell us, stopping the presses and futzing with them just causes additional variation.

Traditional statistical process control of color difference data is dead, Jim!

I should remark that this factor of four is based on one data set. I think it is a good data set, since it is very much real world. But perhaps it includes additional variation because there were 102 printing plants involved? Perhaps there is some idiosyncrasy in newspaper presses? Perhaps there is an idiosyncrasy involved in using the average of all 102 to determine the target color?

I would caution against trying to read too much into the magic factor of four that I arrived at for this data set. But, I will hold my ground and say that the basic principle is sound. Color difference data is not normally distributed, so the basic assumptions about statistical process control are suspect.

In next week's installment of this exciting series, I will investigate the theoretical basis for non-normality of color difference data.

Move on to Part 3

Tuesday, October 18, 2016

Statistical process control of color difference data, part 1

Statistical process control (SPC) of color data -- specifically of color difference (ΔE) data -- can be done, but there is a bit of a twist. Color difference data doesn't behave like your garden variety process control data. Since ΔE doesn't follow the rules, the classical method for computing control limits will no longer work.

In this blog post, I review classical process control to provide a footing for next week's blog, where I pull the rug out from under the footings of the classical approach; explaining why it won't work for color difference measurements. Hopefully, by the time I get around to the third blog post in this trilogy, I will have thought of some new footings on which to erect a new SPC specifically designed for ΔE.

Process control - Do we have an outlier?

Review of process control

The premise of statistical process control is "more or less simple". I say that in the sense that it's not really that simple at all. And I say that because I want to make sure that you understand that what I do is really pretty freaking awesome. But really, the basic idea behind SPC is not all that tough to comprehend: You only investigate your widget-making machine when it starts to produce weird stuff, and you shouldn't sweat it when the product isn't weird.

The complicated part lies in your algorithm for deciding where to draw the line between "normal" and "weird". The red dress on the far left?  Elegant, chic, and attractive, and pretty much in line with what all the women at my widget factory are wearing. The next one over? Yeah... I see her in the cafeteria once in a while. But I'm just not getting into the outfit on the far right. Sorry. I'm just not a fan of horizontal stripes. But in between... how do you decide where to draw the line?

Where to draw the line????

Statistical process control has an answer. You start by characterizing your process. As you manufacture widgets, you pull out samples and measure something about them. Hopefully you measure something that is relevant, like the distance between the threads of a bolt, or the weight of the cereal in the box. Since you are (apparently) reading this blog post, it would seem that the widget's color might be the attribute that interests you.

Next, you sadistically characterize this big pile of data. Open up a spreadsheet, and open up a bottle of Black and Tan, a Killian's Red, a Pale or Brown Ale, a Blue Moon, or an Amber Lager. And unleash the sarcastical analysis.

The goal for your spreadsheet is to come out with two numbers, which we call the upper control limit and the lower control limit. Then when you saunter into work the following day, after recovering from a colorful hangover, you can start using these two numbers on brand new production data. Measure the next widget off the production line. If it falls between the lower control limit and the upper control limit, then relax and pull another Black and Tan out of your toolbox. You can relax cuz you know your process is under control.

The yellow crayon is just a few nanometers short of a full deck

When a part falls outside the control limits, the camera doesn't automatically cut to Tom Hanks saying "Houston, we have a problem". We're not sure just yet whether this is a real problem or a shell-fish-stick anomaly. The important thing is, we start looking for Jim the SOP Guy, since he is the only one in the plant who knows where to find the standard operating procedure for troubleshooting the widget making machine.

Note that I was careful not to start the previous paragraph with "when a part is bad..." Being outside of control limits does not necessarily mean that the part is unacceptable for the person writing out a check for the widgets. Hopefully, the control limits are well within the tolerances that are written into the contract. And hopefully, the control limits that are used on the manufacturing floor were based entirely off data from the process, and the SPC code of ethics has not been sullied by allowing the customer tolerances to be used in place of control limits. That would be icky.

Identifying control limits

But how do we decide what the appropriate control limits are? If we set the control limits too tight, then Jim the SOP Guy never gets time to finish the Blue Moon he opened up for breakfast. And we all know that Jim gets really ornery if his beer gets warm.

You don't want to get Jim the SOP Guy angry!

If on the other hand, we humor Jim the SOP Guy and widen the tolerances to the point where Tom Hanks can fly a lunar lander through them, then we will potentially fail to react when the poor little widget making machine is desperately in need of a little TLC.

So, every time we encounter another measurement of a widget, we are faced with a judgement call. Setting control limits is inherently a judgement call where we balance the risk of wasted time troubleshooting versus missing a machine that's out of whack.

Deming

Why is it so bad to spend a little extra time troubleshooting?  It is, of course, a business expense, but there is an insidious hidden cost to excessive knob gerfiddling. It makes for more variation in the product. If we try to control a process to tighter than it wants to go, we just wind up chasing our tail.

Well, lemme tell ya about when I worked with Deming. This was back in the late 1940's, just after the Great War to End All Wars. Oh wait. That was WW I. Deming did his stuff just after WW II - the Great War After the Great War to End All Wars. I was about negative thirteen years old at the time. A very precocious young lad of negative thirteen, I was. Deming learned me about the difference between normal variation and special cause. Normal variation is the stuff you can expect with your current process. You can't get rid of this without changing your process. Special cause means that something is broke and needs attending to.

Try this joke at home with Riesling and with Kipling!

Deming traveled to Japan after the war to help rebuild their manufacturing system. He did that very well. I mean, very well. Deming became a super-hero for the Japanese in much the same way that I have become a super-hero for my dogs. Except, of course, that the Japanese came to revere Deming.

In a nutshell, Deming preached that all manufacturing processes have a natural random variation. We should seek, over the long run, to minimize this by improving our process. This is important, but it is not the topic of this blog series. I want to concentrate on the day-to-day. In the short run, we need to understand the magnitude of our variation. This is done by collecting data, and applying statistics to it. This is used to identify subsequent parts that fall outside that range. When this happens, there is a call for identifying the special cause, and correcting the issue.

A part is identified as being potentially bad if it is so far from the norm that it is unlikely to have come from the same process. This is important enough to repeat. A part is identified as being potentially bad if the probability of it falling within the established statistical distribution of the process is very small. So, it's all about probabilities.

Enter normality
If  we assume that the underlying distribution is "normal" (AKA a Gaussian or bell curve), then we can readily characterize the likelihood of a part being bad based on the mean and standard deviation of the process. In a normal distribution, 68% of all samples fall within 1 standard deviation of the mean, 95.5% fall within 2 standard deviations of the mean, and 99.74% fall within 3 standard deviations of the mean.

Folks who have taken credit for DeMoivre's invention
So...

The characterizing of our process is pretty simple. You know, when you opened up the spreadsheet and took a long drink of the Amber Lager?  You don't have to tell your boss how simple it is, but here it is for you: Compute the average of the data. That goes in one cell of a spreadhseet. Compute the standard deviation. That goes in a second cell. Then, multiply the standard deviation by the magic number 3. Subtract this product from the mean (third cell in the spreadsheet), and add this product to the mean (fourth cell). This third and fourth cell are the lower and upper control limits, respectively.

If the process produces normal data, and if nothing changes in our process, then 99.74% of the time, the part will be within those control limits. And once every 400 parts, we will find a part that is nothing more than an unavoidable tansistical anomaly.

The big IF

Note the sentence that predicated assigning the numbers to the likelihood of false alarms: If the underlying distribution is normal...

Spoiler alert for next week's blog post. Color difference data is not normal. And by that I mean, it doesn't fit the normal distribution. This messes up the whole probability thing.

Sadly, differences of color don't live in this city!

Here is a scenario that suggests there may be a difficulty. Let's just say for example, that the average of our color difference data is 5 ΔE, and that the standard deviation is 1 ΔE. That puts our lower control limit at 2 ΔE.

Let's say that we happen to pull out a part and the difference between its color and the target color is 1 ΔE. What should we do? Classical control theory says that we need to start an investigation into why this part is outside of the control limits. Something must be wrong with our process! The sky is falling!

But stop and think about it. If the part is within 1 ΔE of the target color, then it's pretty darn good. Everyone should be happy. Classical control theory would lead us to the conclusion that something must be wrong with our process because the part was closer to the target color than is typical!

The obvious solution to this is that we simply ignore the lower control limit. That will avoid our embarrassment when we realize that we fired that incompetent operator for doing too good a job. But, this simple example is a clue that something larger might be amiss. Stay tuned for next week's exciting blog post, where I explain how it is that color difference values are really far from being normally distributed!

Move on to Part 2

Tuesday, October 11, 2016

A backwards optical illusion

I think this is just downright weird. An optical illusion with a twist.

An illusion

I expect many of you may have seen the clever illusion that I show below. The gray rectangle on the left (the one surrounded by white) looks darker than the one on the right.

This illusion is the topic of the day!

Well, it looks that way, but it's not, really. All we need to do is join the two with a bar of the same color and the illusion goes away. Or, if you suspect that I am doing a little creative image editing, then cut two holes in a piece of paper, one for each square.

The illusion de-mystified

A (failed) attempt to explain the illusion

Why does this happen? To shed some light on the illusion (pun intended), I wanted to see if my camera saw the same thing. So, being the clever and resourceful guy that I am, I displayed this image on my computer screen. and took a picture of it with my Canon G10. The picture can be seen below.

What my camera sees when it looks at this illusion

The square on the left (white surround) has average gray values of (42.8, 45.6, 60.5). The one on the right (black surround) has average gray values of 34.4, 37.1, 10.9). The little square on the left has an average difference in brightness of 8.4 in the red channel, 8.4 in the green, and 10.9 in the blue. Aha!! Just as I thought!! The camera is seeing the same thing I am seeing!

(For those of you who noticed a mistake in my reasoning in that last sentence, just sit tight on your hands for a little while. Please don't spoil the surprise for the rest when I reveal the intriguing error.)

Why does the camera not see the two squares the same? In any optical system, be it a camera or an eyeball, there is scattered light - light that doesn't focus just the way we want it to. As a result, light from bright areas in the scene will scatter into dark areas in the scene. We call this, veiling glare.

The picture below is an extreme case of veiling glare. The photos above and below were taken of the same mastiff figurine and with the same camera and lens. In the one below, I fogged up the lens by breathing on it. Never do that, by the way. It will make your mastiff foggy. (I had a mastiff once... Bubba. I miss him.)

When a good dog gets veiling glared

Note that the black background didn't just turn gray. It took on some of the color of the dog. The fawn colored light coming from the fawn colored coat of Bubba should all have been focused on the image of Bubba at the sensor of the camera. But some of it wound up going somewhere else because of the temporary foggy imperfection in the lens.

This is extreme, as I said, but all lenses do this to an extent. A small portion of the average intensity of the image is added to all the pixels. That's veiling glare. But there is also a more localized effect. Going back to the image that my camera took of my computer screen, the gray square that is surrounded by white is made just a tad bit brighter because it is standing near all those other bright pixels. Just like when I stand next to Albert Einstein, Isaac Newton, and John Von Neumann, their brilliance scatters over me and I look so much brighter.

(Just in case you didn't know, John Von Neumann was one of the fathers of the computer, being credited with the idea of a stored program computer. And, an applied mathematician.)

My high school chess club
(from left to right) Newton, Einstein, me, and Von Neumann

So (prepare for the oops), I have just demonstrated that this effect - the effect of the illusion I started with - can be readily seen in images taken with a camera. And the effect is from the scatter that happens in the lens, be it the one in the eye or the one in the camera.

Say what?


Wait a sec. I think that's exactly backwards of what we perceive when we look at the illusion. The gray patch that is surrounded by white actually shines just a bit brighter on the retina, but it looks darker! How can this be?????!!?!

I gave a bit of a clue, when I mentioned my good friends Albert, Isaac, and John. If you happened to overhear me having a conversation with these gents, would you really think of me as being of their caliber? Or rather, would I  appear more dumber, since I would be compared to them?

Take a look again at my chess club picture. Have a close look at me, in the Gold's Gym shirt. I really don't think I have all that bad of a physique. In isolation, one might actually think I am rather buff. But standing next to those other mesomorphs, I am afraid I have to admit that I look like the anti-hero from a Woody Allen movie - a real nebbish.

The thing is, somewhere between the retina and the cognitive part of the brain, it all becomes about comparisons. The gray square on the left is compared to the white that it is next to. Because of that proximity, it is perceived as being darker than it really is. Similarly, the square on the right is perceived as being lighter due to its proximity to the black area.

Judging by the fact that we really can't "turn this effect off" just by thinking about it, the comparison must have been done prior to the signal reaching the cognitive brain. Maybe it's in the rods and cones? Maybe in the neurons? Or maybe in the lower limbic system?  This sounds like a topic for a another blog. Maybe I will reference another blogpost of mine about the famous "what color is the dress" fiasco.

But it is interesting to note that that the cognitive part of the brain does this same trick. The guy in the chess club pic looks like a wimp. I look dumb when you hear me converse with really bright people. The kid at the prep school who grew up in a middle class family feels like he/she had a life of squalor.

Summarizing...

I have described two effects here. First, there is light scattered in the remarkable optical system. This changes the amount of actual light that is registered in the retina. Areas adjacent to bright stuff have the largest effect.

Someplace in the early parts of  the human visual system, there is a larger counter-effect caused by a constant comparison of objects against what is nearby.

And so, we have a simple illusion that has more to it than one would expect.

Tuesday, October 4, 2016

Blues in the night

Fall is upon us. Everyone in Wisconsin is either eagerly anticipating the lovely fall colors, or are dreading the onset of Seasonal Affective Disorder (SAD). I have already blogged about the color science of the autumn leaves, so, it's probably time for me to blog about the eye, color, and it's tawdry relationship with SAD. Along the way, I will talk about zeitgebers and circadian rhythm entrainment, the spectral response of retinal ganglia, and of course, the supra-chiasmatic nucleus! Hot Damn!

Public service announcement: If this is a medical emergency, please hang up and dial 911. If you are looking for some practical advice on dealing with something that is a little more than the winter blues, I would suggest having a look at the blog post from BetterHelp called Seasonal Depression Is More Than Just The Winter Blues. But if you want to understand what happens in your body that makes SAD happen, then please read on!

Are you getting sleepy yet?

Today's sleep-inducing lecture begins with melatonin. I think most everyone has heard of it, and everyone has used it, whether they are aware of it or not. Melatonin is the hormone that sends a wake up call to all the team leaders in our body that it's time to start wrapping up the day's activity. Melatonin makes us sleepy.


You can buy melatonin from your favorite drug store, supermarket, or neighborhood dope peddler. It is inexpensive, you can get it without a prescription, and it helps you get to sleep.

Alternately, you can get it from your local pineal gland. If you are a vertebrate, then it is quite likely that you own one of these hot little jobbies. The pineal gland is located kinda in the middle of your brain. It's just a tiny thing, but it'll put you to sleep faster than reading a blog post about seasonal affective disorder. The pineal gland is where the melatonin comes from. But only when the pineal gland is good and ready to give you that mellowing stuff.


But, how does the pineal gland decide when it's time to play Mr. Sandman?


I don't know these women, but I wish I would have done this video!

Getting in the rhythm 

What we need is a clock for Mr. Pineal. No. Check that. Mr. Pineal needs an alarm clock. No. Check that again. Mr. Pineal would do real well to have what my electrical engineering friends would call a phase locked loop. Essentially, this is an oscillator with the added feature of having an active mechanism to keep the oscillator in sync. In this case, the phase locked loop would keep in sync with the rotation of the Earth. By the way, the name of the phase locked loop for the pineal gland is the "supra-chiasmatic nucleus". (Not to be confused with the super-charismatic nucleus, AKA John the Math Guy.)

The job of the supra-chiasmatic nucleus

How does the SCN get synchronized? What is the signal that serves as a zeitgeber?

Aside: I throw in words like "zeitgeber" to make it sound like I know what I am talking about. I have learned through the years that every profession has a relatively small collection of words that act as passwords to get you in the door. Use them correctly in a sentence, and you get to join the club.

Zeitgeber is, of course, a German word. "Zeit" means "time", and "geber" means "giver". This is the official name of "that thing that keeps you in sync with the diurnal cycle".

There have been a lot of suggestions about what the zeitgeber (or zeitgebers) might be. Some obvious guesses are coffee, activity, loud noises, your spouse snoring, a cold or hot shower, a good breakfast, and social interaction. All of these will help us to wake up.

But, sadly, these are not the most effective way to actually reset our alarm clock. In other words, let's say I were to get a real job, one where I had to actually get up before noon every day. If I stop at Starbucks tomorrow at 6:00 AM and have a mocha with three shot of espresso, it will not make it easier for me to get up the following morning. That suprachiasmatic nucleus just keeps chugging along, thinking that 2:00 AM is a decent time to crawl between the sheets.

It took a lot of research, but eventually Science came to the conclusion that light is the primary zeitgeber.

Do you think it unromantic of me to see this sunrise and say "What a glorious zeitgeber"?

SAD and the clandestine pathway

Scientists found many animals that could easily be entrained to the day with light. Originally, it was thought that humans just didn't work this way. It wasn't until the late 1980s that it was found that 2,500 to 3,500 lux was required to activate the SCN. (A typical indoor setting is only a few hundred lux.)

SAD (so the theory goes) is merely what happens when the body is saying "it's time to sleep" when annoyingly happy people are awake being annoyingly happy. SAD is a failure to entrain the SCN during the winter when there is a dearth of sunlight.

Light therapy with full spectrum lights has been used to treat SAD. Historically, treatment of SAD has taken gobs and gobs of light, basically four fluorescent tubes at arm's length. Researchers found that it took a lot of light to entrain a human, and that the light from an incandescent bulb was not particularly effective. So-called "full spectrum" lights became the recommended therapy. Note that the difference between incandescent and full spectrum light is at the blue end. Full-spectrum light has a lot more blue.

It was only fairly recently (1991) that researchers found that the rods and cones in the eye were not the light receptors that kept the SCN in sync with the day. The actual pathway was through the network of nerves in the eye which is called the retinal ganglion. And guess what? The peak response of these puppies is in the blue region, somewhere between 460 nm and 480 nm. This is blue light, by the way.

Today, you can find light therapy boxes that use the much more efficient blue light to treat SAD. It is perhaps coincidental that there are blue LEDs with a dominant wavelength very close to the peak response of the ganglion.

See how annoyingly happy and productive she is?

Avoiding disentrainment

The Foundation for the Research and Investigation of Early Nighttime Diversions (FRIEND) recently published a study that showed that an alarming number of couples have recently forgone previous snuggling activity in preference to checking email and watching stupid cat videos on their cell phones and tablets. [This study involved a random sampling of couples living in my house. The study was published in a blog post from John the Math Guy, entitled "Blues in the night".]



This is a problem. First off, cuz I like snuggling. But perhaps more importantly, cuz many cellphones emit light in that critical region between 460 nm and 480 nm. In other words, the devices stimulate this pathway that tells the SCN that it is still daytime. My closest friend, Jeff Yurek, published a blog post that says that certain devices are less prone to this problem. [Jeff became one of my closest friends when he posted a link to one of my blog posts. I really am that shallow.]

Jeff says that the blue light from quantum dot displays is a bit further into the violet end of the spectrum than the critical region where the ganglion are sensitive. His article calls out the Vizio RS65-B2 as one TV that has a quantum dot display. Who wouldn't want one of these 65" TVs on teh wall in their bedroom?!?

But if you are thinking of something a bit more portable, I just tested my KindleFire HD, and found that it is probably less prone to messing with circadian rhythms.

By the way, I would be more than happy to test anyone's tablet for how susceptible it is to upsetting your biological clock. Of course, I won't guarantee that I will return the device. If I like it, I might just keep it.